Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: http://bura.brunel.ac.uk/handle/2438/22947
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorLew-Levy, S-
dc.contributor.authorPope, SM-
dc.contributor.authorHaun, DBM-
dc.contributor.authorKline, MA-
dc.contributor.authorBroesch, T-
dc.date.accessioned2021-07-16T11:28:16Z-
dc.date.available2021-11-
dc.date.available2021-07-16T11:28:16Z-
dc.date.issued2021-07-14-
dc.identifier105223-
dc.identifier.citationLew-Levy, S. Pope, S.M., Haun, D.B.M., Kline, M.A. and Broesch, T. (2021) 'Out of the empirical box: A mixed-methods study of tool innovation among Congolese BaYaka forager and Bondongo fisher–farmer children', Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 211, 105223 pp. 1 - 13. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105223.en_US
dc.identifier.issn0022-0965-
dc.identifier.urihttps://bura.brunel.ac.uk/handle/2438/22947-
dc.description.abstract© 2021 The Authors. Tool innovation has played a crucial role in human adaptation. Yet, this capacity seems to arise late in development. Before 8 years of age, many children struggle to solve the hook task, a common measure of tool innovation that requires modification of a straight pipe cleaner into a hook to extract a prize. Whether these findings are generalizable beyond postindustrialized Western children remains unclear. In many small-scale subsistence societies, children engage in daily tool use and modification, experiences that theoretically could enhance innovative capabilities. Although two previous studies found no differences in innovative ability between children from Western and small-scale subsistence societies, these did not account for the latter’s inexperience with pipe cleaners. Thus, the current study investigated how familiarity with pipe cleaners affected hook task success in 132 Congolese BaYaka foragers (57 girls) and 59 Bondongo fisher–farmers (23 girls) aged 4–12 years. We contextualized these findings within children’s interview responses and naturalistic observations of how pipe cleaners were incorporated into daily activities. Counter to our expectation, prior exposure did not improve children’s performance during the hook task. Bondongo children innovated significantly more hooks than BaYaka children, possibly because they participate in hook-and-line fishing. Observations and interviews showed that children imagined and innovated novel uses for pipe cleaners outside the experimental context, including headbands, bracelets, and suspenders. We relate our findings to ongoing debates regarding systematic versus unsystematic tool innovation, the importance of prior experience for the ontogeny of tool innovation, and the external validity of experimental paradigms.en_US
dc.format.extent1 - 13 (13)-
dc.format.mediumPrint-Electronic-
dc.languageen-
dc.publisherElsevier BVen_US
dc.rights© 2021 The Authors. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).-
dc.rights.urihttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/-
dc.subjecttool Innovationen_US
dc.subjecttool makingen_US
dc.subjectproblem solvingen_US
dc.subjectsmall-scale societiesen_US
dc.subjectcross-culturalen_US
dc.subjectcognitive developmenten_US
dc.titleOut of the empirical box: A mixed-methods study of tool innovation among Congolese BaYaka forager and Bondongo fisher–farmer childrenen_US
dc.typeArticleen_US
dc.identifier.doihttps://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2021.105223-
dc.relation.isPartOfJournal of Experimental Child Psychology-
pubs.issueNovember 2021-
pubs.publication-statusPublished online-
pubs.volume211-
dc.identifier.eissn1096-0457-
Appears in Collections:Psychology

Files in This Item:
File Description SizeFormat 
FullText.pdf1.26 MBAdobe PDFView/Open


This item is licensed under a Creative Commons License Creative Commons